Anchoring Bias: Definition and Examples

Reviewed by Patricia Brown

What is Anchoring Bias?

Anchoring bias is a cognitive bias where individuals disproportionately rely on the initial information ("anchor") received when forming judgments or making decisions. This anchor skews subsequent assessments, even in the presence of updated or conflicting data.

Key Insights

  • Anchoring bias causes initial data points to disproportionately influence subsequent judgments and decisions.
  • Structured decision-making techniques and rigorous analytical frameworks reduce anchoring effects.
  • Actively considering alternative data and scenarios mitigates reliance on initial anchors.

Key insights visualization

Anchoring bias significantly impacts decision-making processes in fields such as behavioral economics, finance, negotiation strategies, and market pricing. Recognizing anchoring bias helps organizations develop effective decision frameworks and training programs, leveraging techniques such as independent baselining, scenario planning, and multiple-source validation to limit cognitive distortions. Relevant analytical methods include sensitivity analyses and benchmarking studies, which counteract anchoring bias by encouraging decision-makers to systematically evaluate data from diverse reference points (Behavioral Economics).

Why it happens

Anchoring bias emerges through a mix of mental shortcuts, limited processing resources, and heuristics. People use anchors because starting from a concrete value conserves effort when dealing with complex or ambiguous data. The mind applies minimal adjustment from that starting point, often yielding outcomes skewed in the anchor’s direction.

Initial theories in cognitive psychology attributed anchoring to selective accessibility (selective exposure). Once an anchor is set, the mind selectively retrieves information that reinforces it, creating a subtle confirmation loop. Modern neuroscience research further shows that anchoring involves areas of the brain tied to attention control and memory retrieval, indicating that this bias is deeply embedded at a neurological level.

When confronted with a high anchor, people may inflate their predictions by thinking, "If the anchor is that high, reality could be higher than I thought," thus shifting judgments upward.

Anchoring in negotiation contexts

Many business and legal scenarios revolve around negotiations, where initial offers set a mental boundary for subsequent discussions. Even if an initial request seems extreme, it tends to alter perceived reasonableness moving forward, strongly influencing later counteroffers.

In negotiations, a focal point can emerge from an anchor. When a party starts discussions by demanding a certain sum or establishing a specific goal, the other side subconsciously treats that figure as a reference, even if counteroffers or objective data challenge its accuracy.

Skilled negotiators often place strong anchors early to set expectations and create advantages. Both sides benefit when they recognize these anchors, carefully scrutinizing their basis and actively challenging assumptions stemming from them.

Anchoring in consumer decisions

Shoppers regularly face pricing strategies that exploit anchoring. Stores use high prices or exaggerated discounts to establish initial anchors, making subsequent prices appear more attractive—even if that "sale" price remains higher than fair market value. These tactics are common in loyalty programs, product bundles, and seasonal sales.

Retailers often display a "was" price alongside a "now" price, anchoring customers to the higher initial figure. Even if the original price is inflated, consumers feel they're getting a deal and may disregard critical evaluation of product value. Furthermore, when sale countdown clocks add time pressure, urgency combined with anchoring can override rational decision-making processes, prompting impulsive purchases.

Case 1 – Real estate listing prices

Real estate markets offer prominent examples of anchoring bias in action. Property listing prices act as powerful anchors, shaping buyers' perceptions of fair value. Even when provided comparisons with similar properties, buyers often anchor on the original asking price, leading to final prices influenced strongly by initial listings.

Homebuyers usually start searching with a firm budget in mind, but an initial high-priced listing can introduce doubt, prompting questions like, "Could it really be worth that much?" As a result, negotiations may end at figures reflecting the anchor rather than objective property assessments.

The appraisal gap

Real estate appraisals attempt to ground home prices in objective data. Nevertheless, appraisers can themselves fall prey to anchoring if they are aware of the listing price before making their valuation. Consequently, artificially high listing prices can result in inaccurately high appraisals, further perpetuating anchoring effects within the property market.

Case 2 – Salary negotiations

Salary discussions between job candidates and employers often revolve around anchors that shape expectations. When a candidate starts with a high anchor, subsequent offers from employers frequently cluster near that number. Alternatively, when a hiring manager introduces a lower compensation figure, the negotiation may settle at an amount below the candidate's potential market worth.

While many job seekers hesitate to anchor first—out of fear of appearing aggressive—a carefully researched and presented salary range can establish a strong and justified anchor. Doing so prevents employers from setting a low anchor and potentially limiting earnings.

Understanding how anchors persist

In salary negotiations, both candidates and employers provide new data, including the candidate's experience, job responsibilities, and market comparisons. Despite these objective details, the initial anchor endures as a psychological reference point, affecting expectations throughout the negotiation, even if it doesn't accurately reflect real market conditions.

Origins

Anchoring bias emerged from early research by psychologists Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman. Their pioneering work in the 1970s demonstrated that people often generate numerical estimates by starting at an initial value and inadequately adjusting away from that anchor. Their discoveries significantly influenced the development of behavioral economics, providing a foundation for understanding decision-making anomalies.

Tversky and Kahneman's research highlighted various cognitive shortcuts, demonstrating clearly that individuals frequently diverge from purely rational decision-making. Anchoring bias proved to be robustly replicable across diverse scenarios, highlighting its relevance to disciplines like finance, law, and public policy.

Economist Herbert Simon contributed further insights by introducing the concept of bounded rationality. Under conditions of limited cognitive resources, humans rely on simplified mental strategies, among which anchoring serves as a primary example. Through anchoring, complex decisions become manageable, though this simplicity can lead to systematic errors.

Anchoring interacts with other cognitive biases, which can amplify or perpetuate its effects. Confirmation bias supports anchoring as individuals selectively seek information confirming their anchored reference points. Meanwhile, overconfidence bias leads people to believe their initially anchored judgments are accurate, discouraging adjustments based on additional evidence.

BiasCore MechanismInteraction with Anchoring
Anchoring biasOverreliance on the first piece of infoSets an initial numerical reference
Confirmation biasFocus on information confirming existing beliefsReinforces anchor by disregarding conflicting information
Overconfidence biasOverestimation of one's accuracy or knowledgePrevents re-evaluation of anchored judgments

How these biases accumulate

When anchoring combines with confirmation bias and overconfidence, individuals become highly resistant to new information and alternative perspectives. An initial anchor can thus set the stage for ongoing errors in judgment. This cumulative effect is evident in corporate project estimates or budgeting, where early projections can skew later assessments and remaining decisions over time.

FAQ

Is anchoring bias always harmful?

No, anchoring bias isn’t inherently harmful in every scenario. Anchors grounded in accurate, objective data can serve as helpful baselines, guiding informed decisions. However, hidden or arbitrary anchors—created by guesses, emotions, or manipulations—can cause systematic errors, leading to irrational and costly outcomes.

How can one spot an anchor before it influences decisions?

Anchors typically emerge as initial figures or statements that frame subsequent discussion points. Identifying an anchor early involves carefully scrutinizing any baseline numbers or assumptions introduced at the outset. Once recognized, evaluate the anchor's validity by comparing alternative sources or different perspectives.

Can technology-based solutions remove anchoring bias altogether?

Automated or algorithmic systems can significantly reduce anchoring bias by performing objective, impartial calculations. However, if human-generated inputs set initial reference figures (anchors), bias can persist or even introduce new forms of anchoring. Awareness, training, and human oversight remain critical.

End note

Organizations and individuals who understand the power of anchoring can better safeguard their negotiations, estimations, and everyday decisions. Proper management of this bias contributes to more balanced and rational outcomes in both professional and personal life. Continuous reflection on anchoring’s influence encourages clearer judgment amid a world full of data and competing viewpoints.

Share this article on social media